Review: Human
Development and the Spiritual Life: How Consciousness Grows toward
Transformation
by Ronald
R. Irwin
Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers: New York, etc. 2002
Michael Lamport Commons Joel Funk
Harvard Medical School Plymouth
State College
The field of positive
adult development began in the mid-1970's but really became an organized field
in 1981, with the first symposium on postformal
thinking, entitled Beyond Formal Operations. Despite this apparently long history,
the overall field of adult development is only coming to recognize the
importance of higher stages of development, whether cognitive, moral, egoic, or spiritual. One curious characteristic of many
individuals who do research on higher stages of development is their strong
interest in the growth and development of consciousness and spirituality (e.g. Miller & Gook-Greuter,
1994; Miller & West, 2000; Sinnott, 2000, 2001). This is perhaps due
to the growing interest in Eastern
thought emerging from the human potential movement. Irwin’s book reviews many of the
theories of the adult development of self and consciousness first proposed in
the 1980's in lucid, but non-integrative terms. In addition, Irwin presents his own
views about development.
To do so, he synthesizes three perspectives: an evolutionary
perspective, a dialectical and stage perspective, and an Eastern and
spirituality perspective.
As such, his thinking is representative of one of the threads of
adult development.
This book is divided
into two main sections: 1) Irwin’s general outline of a theory of psychological
development, and 2) “his sketch of a theory of the nature of self and spiritual
development from both Eastern (Oriental-Asiatic) and Western (Euro-American)
perspectives. One
of the more interesting sections is Irwin's discussion of “ego and self” in
terms of evolution, childhood and ego development, impulse control and
repression, cognition, and language. Irwin’s evolutionary theory has it
that we evolve to conform–a surprising argument at first glance. It is true that
most people terminate development in one of the conventional stages (e.g.,
Concrete, Abstract, Formal, see Table 1), but there are many well known
examples of highly developed individuals who move beyond the conventional
levels and conformity.
Irwin’s model may have trouble accounting for the
post-conventional stages.
In Part 2, the author
delves into identity: identity as development and achievement, as fiction and constraint,
and as narrative. Irwin
sees the narrative approach as crucial for understanding the self. The author
identifies three types of knowing: epistemic knowing, relativistic knowing,
dialectical knowing, and then turns to
what he call self-knowing.
In this subsection of Part 2 appear discussions of Kegan’s vertical-organizational changes in ego, of midlife
crisis, of Pascual-Leone’s moral and social knowing ,
and the substantial contributions to this field on the parts of Kohlberg and Armon.
Under “cosmic knowing,” which focuses on spiritual development,
Irwin considers the work of Cook-Greuter, Wilber,
Alexander, and Fowler, among others.
Unfortunately, Irwin
leaves the reader with the impression that these are really separate theories
rather than flavors of the same developmental sequence with different content
as shown in Table 1. In
the integrative view, there can only be one set of stages of the development of
self and consciousness.
There can only be one stage sequence and the higher stages must
be defined in terms of and organize lower stage actions and constructions in a
non-arbitrary way. Compared
to Wilber's (2000) model or the concordance shown in
Table 1, Irwin’s review seems somewhat out of date and lacking in power and
scope. Also,
the more scientific stage models of Commons et al., Dawson, and Fischer are not
reviewed. Because
those theories are not strictly theories of the development of consciousness,
perhaps, he does not include them. As a result, he misses an opportunity
to integrate these disparate theories in the manner of Wilbur (2002) and Miller and Cook-Greuter
(1994).
Other issues raised by
Irwin remain somewhat problematic. Theories of spiritual development tend
to fall into two camps: one suggests that higher states consciousness or
spiritual development occur after the normal postformal
stages (or at least after one of the higher stages such as metasystematic);
and the other suggests a parallel development of higher states of consciousness. That is,
spirituality is seen as a separate line of development, alongside mathematic,
logical, social and moral judgement and problem
solving. A
major issue remains is whether “spiritual development: comes “on top of “
normal postformal cognitive development (Alexander,
Cook-Greuter, and Kohlberg) or whether it is a
parallel line (Commons, Fowler, Funk and Irwin). Wilber (2000) argues that either view is acceptable
(in fact he offers five definitions of spirituality, several of which do not
involve stages). The
implications of these competing views are not discussed by Irwin.
The first reviewer
observes that many transcendental consciousness theories place the development
of spirituality after one of the higher adult developmental stages. This might happen
because the highest stages of adult development are very difficult to
understand. It
also might happen because very few individuals reason at those higher stages. Hence, there is the
perception of some theoreticians that transcendental theories are similarly ‘high
up.’ However, they do not carry out the kind of reasoning that would be
required to in fact show that their transcendental stages are ‘higher’ than
adult postformal stages. Because it is all so mysterious to
them, they assert that the stage is transcendental and therefore at the ‘top’,
as it were, of adult development. This resembles a common phenomenon
observed in human cultures which is that whenever there is a phenomenon that
people do not understand they attribute some kind of religious/transcendental
explanation to it.
Incidently,
in the generalities that are the meat of the text of this book, lie at least
one potential trouble spot.
Care must be taken with the author’s statements on some
peripheral matters. In
the section on language, for example, the author gets basic little things wrong. For example, he misdefines orthography and phonology, and seems totally unaware
of distinctive-feature theory, an important feature of linguistics for more
than 30 years now, when he tells us that the phoneme is the smallest linguistic
unit.
Despite these flaws,
and because of the
excellent writing style and the extensive surveys of existing theories of
transcendental development, this book would be a useful introduction to at
least some of the historical theories of the development of consciousness for
advanced undergraduates, graduate students and professionals who are interested
in positive adult development.
References
Commons, M. L.,
Richards, F. A., & Armon, C. (Eds.). (1984). Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and
adult cognitive development. NY: Praeger.
Miller, M. E., & Cook-Greuter, S. R. (Eds.). (1994). Transcendence and mature thought in adulthood : the further reaches of adult development. Lanham, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield.
Miller,
M. E., & West, A. N. (Eds.). (2000). Spirituality, ethics, and
relationship in adulthood: Clinical and theoretical explorations. Madison, CT:
Psychosocial Press.
Fowler, J. (1994). Moral stages and
the development of faith. In Puka, Bill (Ed). Fundamental research in
moral development. Moral development: A compendium, Vol. 2. (pp.
344-374) New York: Garland Pub..
Sinnott,
J. D. (2001). A time for the condor and the eagle to fly
together: Relations between spirit and
adult development in healing techniques in several cultures. Journal of Adult Development.
8(4), 241-247. Special Issue
Wilber, K. (2000). Integral
Psychology: Consciousness, spirit, psychology, therapy. Boston: Shambhala. Publications.
Table
Comparative Table Postformal
Stage
Researchers |
Abstract |
Formal |
Systematic |
Meta-systematic |
Paradigm-atic |
Cross-Paradigmatic |
Transcen-dental |
Transcendental 2 |
Commons & Richards (1984) |
9. variables formed from concrete classes |
10. relations formed from abstract variables |
11. systems
formed from formal-operational
relations |
12. meta-systems formed from systems |
13. paradigms formed from metasystems |
14. paradigms crossed to form new fields |
|
|
Inhelder
& Piaget (1958) |
formal III-A |
formal III-B |
consolidated form, postformal |
polyvalent logic; systems of systems |
|
|
|
|
Kohlberg (1981) |
3 mutuality |
3/4 |
4 social system |
5 prior rights/ social
contract 6 universal ethical
principles |
Stage 7 transcen-dental |
|
|
|
Fowler
(1994) |
3 |
3/4 |
4 |
5 |
|
|
|
|
Armon (1984) |
3 affective mutuality |
3/4 |
4 individuality |
5 autonomy |
6 universal categories |
|
|
|
Labouvie-Vief (1984) |
|
intra-systematic |
inter-systematic |
autonomous |
|
|
|
|
Sinnott (1984) |
|
formal |
relativistic/ relativize
systems, metalevel rules |
unified theory: interpretation of contradictory
levels |
|
|
|
|
Basseches (1984) |
phase 1b: formal early foundations |
phase 2 intermediate
dialectical schemes |
phase 3: 2 out of 3 clusters of advanced dialectical
schemes |
4. advanced dialectical thinking |
|
|
|
|
King & Kitchener (2002) |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
|
|
|
|
Benack (1994) |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
|
|
|
|
Pascual-Leone (1983) |
late concrete |
formal and late concrete |
pre-dialectical |
dialectical |
|
|
transcendental |
|
Kegan (1994) |
3 interpersonal |
3/4 |
4 institutional |
5 |
|
|
|
|
Loevinger (1998) |
conformist-conscientious |
conscientious |
individualistic |
autonomous integrated |
|
|
|
|
Cook-Greuter (1990) |
3/4 |
4 |
4/5 |
5 |
5/6 |
6 |
states of consciousness |
|
Journal of Adult Development........................................................................................................................................ 3
Miller
& Gook-Greuter, 1994.......................................................................................................................................... 1
Miller
& West, 2000........................................................................................................................................................ 1
Miller
and Cook-Greuter (1994)...................................................................................................................................... 1
Sinnott, 2000, 2001. ....................................................................................................................................................... 1
Wilber
(2000) ............................................................................................................................................................. 2,
6
Wilber's
(2000................................................................................................................................................................. 1
Index